Going In Through The Back Door: Challenging Straight Male Homohysteria And Transphobia Through Receptive Penetrative Sex Toy Use
A SCHOLARLY PAPER FROM THE GRIEVANCE STUDIES PROJECT
Summary
Published in Sexuality & Culture, a highly regarded journal in sexualities studies.
In the name of M Smith, MA (fictitious) of the (fictitious) Portland Ungendering Research (PUR) Institute by Peter Boghossian, James Lindsay, and Helen Pluckrose.
Discipline/subdiscipline: sexualities studies
Summary: That it is suspicious that straight men rarely anally self-penetrate using sex toys, and that this is probably due to fear of being thought homosexual (“homohysteria”) and bigotry against trans people (transphobia). (It combines these ideas into a novel concept “transhysteria,” which was suggested by one of the paper’s peer reviewers.) Encouraging them to engage in receptive penetrative anal eroticism will decrease transphobia and increase feminist values.
Purpose: That journals will accept ludicrous arguments if they support (unfalsifiable) claims that common (and harmless) sexual choices made by straight men are actually homophobic, transphobic, and anti-feminist.
Notes on Status:
Accepted and published by Sexuality & Culture
Accepted: June 9, 2018.
Published online: June 16, 2018.
Link: https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12119-018-9536-0
NB: We cited this paper in another of our accepted papers, the “Hooters” paper. This indicates how nonsense in one paper can propagate into other papers and, over time, skew the canon of literature in these fields.
Selected Reviewer Comments:
“This article is an incredibly rich and exciting contribution to the study of sexuality and culture, and particularly the intersection between masculinity and anality. … This contribution, to be certain, is important, timely, and worthy of publication .” -Reviewer 1, Sexuality and Culture
“Sorry for so many questions, but this paper is so rich and exciting, I’m just overwhelmed by so many new questions—which is a sign of a marvelous paper!” -Reviewer 1, Sexuality and Culture
“Overall, this paper is a very interesting contribution to knowledge.” -Reviewer 1, Sexuality and Culture
Contents:
Abstract
Introduction
Male Anal Pleasure
Sex Toys and Straight Anal Eroticism
Relationship of Anal Eroticism to Other Straight Masculinity Variables
Methodology
Responses
Discussion
Compliance with Ethical Standards
References
Reviewer Comments
Abstract
To date, very little research literature exists concerning receptive penetrative anal eroticism in straight men. Of particular interest are its impacts upon other factors relevant to masculinities, sex roles, and the study of sexualities. Several co-constituted features of masculinity are likely to be relevant to straight-male anal sexuality, including masturbatory play with penetrative sex toys. Specifically, this study seeks to explore, “Do men who report greater comfort with receptive penetrative anal eroticism also report less transphobia, less obedience to masculine gender norms, greater partner sensitivity, and greater awareness about rape?” This study uses semistructured interviews with thirteen men to explore this question, analyzed with a naturalist and constructivist grounded theory approach in the context of sexualities research and introduces transhysteria as a parallel concept to Anderson’s homohysteria. This analysis recognizes potential socially remedial value for encouraging male anal eroticism with sex toys.
Introduction
While much scholarly work in the study of sexualities has explored the sexual practices of homosexuals and heterosexuals, the topic of straight men who engage in receptive anal pleasure has received limited attention (e.g., Heywood and Smith 2014). In fact, the “first-ever examination of how and how often heterosexual undergraduate men in the United States practice receptive anal eroticism” was only published in 2017 (Branfman et al. 2017, 2). There, Branfman et al. re-examined a blend
of quantitative and qualitative survey data of undergraduate men to identify and understand the relationships some straight men have with receptive anal eroticism. They interpreted their data through theoretical lenses, including Anderson’s homosexualization (2008) and homohysteria (2009), and called for a broader study into the anal erotic practices of straight men (Branfman et al. 2017, 2, 15).
This paper does not seek to satisfy Branfman’s et al. specific call for longitudinal data on male anal erotic practices (Branfman et al. 2017, 2, 15). Instead, it draws inspiration from their inquiry and applies qualitative methodology to explore the questions: “Why don’t straight men (tend to) use penetrative sex toys on themselves to experience (anal) sexual pleasure?” and “What might change if they did?” More formally stated, this study seeks to open exploration into a related question with a small group of men: “Do men who report greater comfort with receptive penetrative anal eroticism also report less transphobia, less obedience to masculine gender norms, greater partner sensitivity, and greater awareness about rape?” In such, it also develops a notion of (straight male) transhysteria in parallel to Anderson’s concept of homohysteria and recognizes a potential political divide between Anderson’s (2009) inclusive masculinity and Connell’s (1995) hegemonic masculinity.
To consider these questions, this study interrogates straight male sexuality through qualitative analysis of semi-structured interviews with thirteen men, most of whom are straight. These interviews seek to identify features of male sexuality as they intersect with masculinity, heteronormativity and homophobia, partner sensitivity, rape culture, attitudes about feminism, and transhysteria/phobia. Ultimately, in addition to adding depth to the questions surrounding (straight) male anal sexuality, this paper explores whether advocacy for and education about straight male penetrative sex toy use could improve expressions of masculinity relevant to these variables. Of course, this sample is small and should not be understood to establish concrete conclusions on these issues. However, the in-depth, interactive approach enabled by working with a small sample suggested interesting and theoretically sound connections which future researchers could pursue.
Though Allan (2016a, 2016b) lays out psychoanalytic theoretical and other considerations that are strongly suggestive of co-constitutive relationships between masculinity vis-à-vis those variables listed above and anality (cf. Pronger 1998), currently there is virtually no scholarly literature that engages the topic of straight male penetrative sex toy directly and substantively. This does not imply the topic is never broached. As noted by Branfman et al. (2017, 2),
Relatively few academic studies have examined how men view, practice, and experience receptive anal pleasure …. Those studies that do investigate male anal eroticism have largely focused on same-sex penile-anal intercourse, usually ignoring how men might receive anal pleasure during heterosexual play, as well as ignoring all forms of anal stimulation without a penis.
Among these, Branfman and Ekberg-Stiritz (2012) approach the topic in a theoretical and historical way. As summarized by Branfman et al.:
Using feminist and queer theory to analyze the social stigmas around men’s anal pleasure, the authors argue that educators can employ the topic of male anal pleasure to help students critically analyze the social construction of sex, gender and sexuality. (Branfman et al. 2017, 3).
Far more extensive and applicable treatments exist in The Ultimate Guide to Prostate Pleasure (Glickman and Emirzian 2013) and The Adventurous Couple’s Guide to Strap–On Sex (Blue 2007), but as these insightful volumes fall outside of the scholarly canon, not all scholars take them as seriously as they deserve. Finally, although these sources utilize social constructivist models, there remains a conspicuous gap in the research literature. Nothing to date applies a constructivist grounded theory approach to this dimension of sexualities studies in order to interpret (reluctance about) straight male penetrative sex toy use and its relationships with other salient variables relevant to straight masculinities (e.g., partner sensitivity and homo/transphobia). This paper seeks to fill that gap.
Male Anal Pleasure
Cultural perceptions of receptive anal eroticism for straight men tend to be hegemonic (that is, legitimizing the dominant patriarchal order) and negative. In fact, the male anus is generally considered sexually taboo within straight sexuality. Even after advances in “inclusive” masculinities (Anderson 2009), receptive anal eroticism primarily conjures themes from hegemonic masculinity theory, including concerns of “being gay” and co-constituent affective associations that run counter to straight masculinities (Kimmel 2001). While heteronormativity and even homophobia serve as the major underpinnings for these responses (e.g., Allan 2016a; Pronger 1998, 1999), anxieties rooted in Anderson’s more refined concept, “homosexualization” (2008), apply more thoroughly. Under homosexualization, “certain activities are coded as ‘gay’ and hence can throw a man’s heterosexual identity and reputation into question” (Branfman et al. 2017, 4). This includes receptive anal eroticism, which accompanies “the cultural belief that straight men who stimulate their own anus, or willingly allow another to stimulate it, must really be gay” (p. 4). Specifically, they posit,
Culturally, however, there exists a wide assumption that only gay and bisexual men desire or receive anal pleasure. These stereotypes about the male prostate are characterized by two key ideas … [including] that a man who enjoys receptive anal pleasure is socially perceived as gay and/or emasculated. Even as anal pleasure is stigmatized by its connotations of homosexuality and feminization, so in turn are gay and bisexual men frequently denigrated as dirty, emasculated, or deviant precisely for their cultural association with anal eroticism. (Branfman et al. 2017, 2).
In response, straight men often exhibit what Anderson (2009) terms “homohysteria,” which he defines as “heterosexual men’s fear of being perceived as gay, especially when they transgress masculine gender norms” (Branfman et al. 2017, 2). Anderson (2009) describes homohysteria as a kind of “social paranoia” that straight men feel results from a loss of masculinity if associated with or identified as “gay” (cf. Allan 2016b). Thus, to avoid associations with homosexuality, straight men rigidly close off to receptive anal eroticism (cf. Allan 2016b; Connell 1995). Branfman et al. (2017, 5) explain,
This is to say that the social stigma on male homosexuality also limits the sexual and gendered lives of heterosexual men …. Just as gay men must avoid certain feminized behaviours if they desire to be thought socially heterosexual, so too must straight men.
These findings comport with Kimmel’s (2001, 33) observation that masculinity is so intrinsically linked to homophobia that homophobia itself contributes to the development of masculine gender identity through fear, shame, and silence. Indeed, Pascoe (2007) argues that the term “fag” is frequently used to enforce heteronormative, even homophobic masculinity in adolescent boys, who build their masculinity by repudiating a “fag” identity. Further, McNeil (2013) documented how these themes are routinely reproduced in sex education; Burke (2014) identified them within conservative Christian religious practices; and Frank (2008) found them reinforced in (sexually) swinging communities. This all agrees with Pronger (1999), who observed that the homo/heterosexual boundary for straight men is performative and rigidly enforced despite, in reality, being far more complicated than most straight masculinities permit. This creates a marked pressure within straight men to conform to sexual practices that exclude receptive anal play. To address and remediate this problem, scholarship has attempted to decode the processes by which categories of sex, gender, and sexuality are constructed and legitimized, particularly with regard to heteronormativity and trans-exclusivity (Carrera et al. 2012). Still, these problems persist and remain understudied.
Returning to Anderson (2009), homohysteria almost undoubtedly contributes to the taboo sexual status of the straight male anus, for this is the role taboos play in social construction: to enforce boundaries that limit disruptive interaction with that which cultures viscerally fear. Conceptually, homohysteria naturally generalizes, with the same straight-male paranoia as its source, to transhysteria, which can be defined in parallel as heterosexual men’s fear of losing masculinity as a result of (potentially) accepting trans women’s penises. Applying to both concepts, as Branfman et al. (2017, 5) write, “The desire to be perceived as heterosexual and masculine is understandable in a culture that distributes privilege unequally according to gender and sexuality.”
The data presented by Branfman et al. (2017) support Anderson’s hypothesized roles of homohysteria, homosexualization, and transhysteria in shaping straight male anal sexuality:
We argue that a man’s socially perceived heterosexual identity is partially conditioned not only upon sex with “appropriate” (opposite-sex) partners, but also upon “appropriate” sex roles. According to this social norm, heterosexual men who wish to avoid stigma must penetrate women, not stimulate or penetrate their own orifices, or allow their orifices to be stimulated or penetrated by others—even if those others are women. While stimulating one’s own anus is not direct sexual contact with another male, homohysteria constructs anal stimulation as a homosexual affair, even if performed in the absence of another male. (p. 6).
These problems therefore exclude receptive anal eroticism—especially penetrative anal eroticism—from the permissible sexual repertoires of straight men that create an explicit tension (located in the straight male anus) between inclusive masculinity and themes of hegemonic masculity (Branfman et al. 2017; cf. Connell 1995). As characterized by Connell, hegemonic masculinity is “the configuration of gender practice which embodies the currently accepted answer to the problem of legitimacy of patriarchy, which guarantees … the dominant position of men and the subordination of women” (1995, 77). More simply, it “embodies a ‘currently accepted’ strategy” for legitimizing patriarchy and represents the “one form of masculinity rather than others [that] is currently exalted” (p. 77). Particularly, for the anal-unreceptive straight man, penetrative sexual acts are frequently seen performatively as something to be done by the man through insertion of his penis into (cisgendered) women.
Not all straight sexual activity, however, relies upon this characterization (cf. Ward 2015). Glickman and Emirzian (2013, 268) observe,
[T]he idea that in sex men have one role and women have another is very restrictive. Men should do all the giving and women should do all the receiving: these roles work fine if your sex life consists only of fucking with the man controlling all the motion. But a lot of people with very satisfying sex lives have roles that are more blurred.
These observations are consistent with the ways in which hegemonic masculinity is interactionally constituted and at odds with inclusive masculinity, especially in situations exhibiting low partner sensitivity (Connell 1995; Schrock and Padavic 2007). Anal-unreceptive straight men do not limit the “doingness” of their sex to penile-vaginal intercourse, however. They often exhibit little hesitance to, or even overt enthusiasm about, inserting their penises into a woman’s anus, about which women report a variety of opinions (cf. McBride 2017). Thus, straight men reproduce hegemonic norms of male sexual dominance by inserting their penises into women’s bodies, and expectations about masculinity (compelled ultimately by homohysteria and transhysteria) dictate and enforce a double-standard regarding anal penetration. That is, although many other forms of anality and anal play exist and interact with masculinity, for many straight men, anal penetration specifically is something that they do (to cisgendered women) but not something that can be done to them (by anyone, man, woman, or self).
An interesting partial exception exists to this anal-exclusive rule among straight men: straight male sexuality may not be a wholly rigid category, especially regarding homoeroticism. Some men describe themselves as “mostly straight” (Savin-Williams and Vrangalova 2013; Ward 2015). This phenomenon demonstrates the intrinsic elasticity of nominally straight male sexuality, which has been explored further under the rubric of “straight with a pinch of bi” (Carrillo and Hoffman 2017). For instance, as documented by Silva, some straight men engage in anal intercourse and other normatively homosexual sex acts as “bud-sex,” highlighting the overall “flexibility of heterosexuality” as a category (2017, 51). These subcategories within nominally straight male sexuality reveal the category as less well-defined than generally assumed (per Carrillo and Hoffman 2017; Silva 2017) and show that heteronormativity often forces bisexual men to perform as straight, to the point of lying (even to themselves) about their sexualities. Nevertheless, sexuality-flexible “straightness” in males may productively challenge orthodoxies about heterosexuality (cf. Beasley et al. 2015) in a playful way (Paasonen 2017) and provide a fitting context into which this research can be theoretically situated.
Sex Toys and Straight Anal Eroticism
One straightforward potential remedy to straight male anxiety about receptive penetrative anal eroticism arrives through experiencing and overcoming these affective responses in a safe, controlled environment. Such “exposure therapy” can successfully address phobic anxieties by inducing carefully controlled exposure to anxiety triggers in safe environments (cf. Foa et al. 1999). Reasonably, then, straight men could overcome some homo/transhysterical anxiety about receptive penetrative anal eroticism in safe “straight” environments through penetrative anal masturbation with sex toys and/or pegging (allowing the female in a straight sexual encounter to anally penetrate the male with a strap-on dildo) with a compassionate partner (cf. Allan 2016b).
There is little scholarly literature available about (straight) male sex toy use, however, as most sex-toy studies explore women’s issues (see Attwood 2005). Few papers exist on the subject, and these typically focus upon introducing sex toys— usually for women—into heterosexual couples’ sexual play (e.g., Watson et al. 2015). For insight, then, popular guides such as Glickman and Emirzian (2013) and Blue (2007) offer clues. Glickman and Emirzian (2013, esp. 198–232) indicate that there are many reasons for men of all sexual orientations (especially straight) to consider receptive anal eroticism, especially penetration (cf. Allan 2016b). This practice can be enjoyable, they contend, and is becoming more commonplace, including by men in heterosexual partnerships. It thus holds transformative potential that can enrich heterosexual relationships, especially when utilized together via pegging of a straight man by a female partner (pp. 233–256) (cf. Allan 2016b; Blue 2007). To facilitate this potential, they address common straight male concerns about homosexualization via penetrative anality:
Receiving penetration is sometimes thought of as the woman’s role in sex. And since a big part of being a “real man” means that you don’t do anything womanly, then of course receiving penetration doesn’t fit. Some men fear that if they get penetrated, they are being feminized—turned into a sissy. (Glickman and Emirzian 2013, 267).
The relationship between anal eroticism and homoeroticism is such a dominant trope in masculinity that Glickman and Emirzian repeatedly return to it (cf. Allan 2016b; Blue 2007, 9–10). Similarly, they engage other issues relevant to the collision of (usually straight) masculinity and receptive penetrative anal eroticism, such as those impacting partner sensitivity, women’s issues (thus feminism), and trans issues (e.g., pp. 18, 98, 182, 258, 271, 291) (cf. Allan 2016b, 185; Blue 2007, 62).
Relationship of Anal Eroticism to Other Straight Masculinity Variables
Theoretical considerations, especially Connell’s (1995) work on hegemonic masculinity as reflected off Anderson’s (2009) inclusive masculinity, lead us to expect that several masculinity variables interact with (straight) male anality and receptive anal penetration. Despite inclusive advances, themes currently dominant in masculinity include problematic trends in partner sensitivity, rape culture, attitudes about feminism, and homo/transhysteria/phobia in straight men, which may have direct or indirect dependence upon attitudes regarding receptive anal eroticism. Connell, for instance, notes that homosexuality, which is connected with anal penetration in many straight masculinities, represents that which is “the repository of whatever is symbolically expelled from hegemonic masculinity” (1995, 78).
Consider “partner sensitivity,” which refers to the capacity for and performance of sympathetic and empathetic behaviors in one member of a partnership for the other(s), such that they demonstrate awareness of the physical, emotional, and mental states and needs of the other(s). It is so common as to be a trope that straight men in heterosexual (dyadic) partnerships tend to lack in this dimension. Consequences range from frustration to failed partnerships (cf. Burke 2014). More commonly, they include internalized sexism (for the woman) and reinforcement of Connell’s (1995; Connell and Messerschmidt 2005) notions of hegemonic masculinity (for the man), reproducing gendered stereotypes, and routine shifts of emotional (and household) labor (onto the woman).
Empirical data gathered by Branfman et al. (2017) bear out some of these observations (cf. Allan 2016b). For example, one of their subjects reported, “Definitely more trust involved in asking to be played with anally than there is in asking to handcuff her or experiment with other kinks” (p. 12). Another noted, “I’m learning what I like in much the same way that females go through the process of learning how they like their clitoris stimulated” (p. 11). Their data suggest that sensitivity to issues relevant to women and women’s sexuality—especially including partner sensitivity, attitudes about feminism, and awareness of rape culture—are likely to be positively impacted by receptive penetrative anal eroticism in straight men (Allan 2016b, esp. 177, 186).
Still, virtually no rigorous scholarly work investigates the topic of directly improving straight male partner sensitivity by means of receptive anal eroticism. Glickman and Emirzian (2013), however, argue persuasively that the practice of “pegging” can lead to remarkable increases in partner sensitivity for recipient males. They articulate that when engaging with receptive penetrative anal eroticism, many men experience “the same variety of emotions as women, and learning to deal with them works a lot better than pretending they don’t happen” (Glickman and Emirzian 2013, 94). They also observe that the male experience of feeling something enter your body is often novel for straight men and explain that learning the ways in which “your mood, your emotions, and your physical sensations in that moment” can have dramatic impacts on the quality of the sexual experience (p. 147) (cf. Allan 2016b, 185). Experiences of these kinds are common for those familiar with receptive sex roles, including women and men who are not straight, but they are less common within straight men, for whom the anus is typically (homohysterically) taboo. In these ways, at a minimum, receptive penetrative anal eroticism in straight men may be hypothesized to facilitate greater partner sensitivity.
These issues immediately tip into the broader realm of straight male attitudes about feminism and rape culture, and much has been written on the intersection of these variables with straight male sexuality (e.g., Baker 2015; Blozendahl and Myers 2004; Canan et al. 2016; Glick and Fiske 2001; Hayes et al. 2016; Phillips 2017; Reling et al. 2017; Rentschler 2014). Here, a thorough rehashing of that literature is omitted, though it bears mentioning that Baker (2015) investigates the role empathy plays in connecting masculinity and attitudes about rape culture. Her conclusion is consistent with the preceding theoretical developments concerning partner sensitivity. Specifically, even without extending from parallel research regarding women’s sensitivity to these issues (Osman 2016), there are excellent reasons to entertain the hypothesis that receptive penetrative anal eroticism in straight men (either in masturbation or with a partner by means of pegging) can introduce relevant empathygenerating lived experiences. These, in turn, can improve attitudes about feminism and concern about rape culture.
Core among the myriad concerns raised by the collision of straight male sexuality with straight male anal-sexual paranoia are transhysteria and transphobia, and these linger even in “inclusive” masculinities. One of the clearest links between straight male transphobia/hysteria and homophobia/hysteria was developed by Nagoshi et al. (2008), who performed a detailed study linking transphobia and homophobia across multiple demographics. They found that these are, indeed, linked, especially in (straight) men. Building on Lombardi et al. (2002), who documented that more than half of trans individuals experience pervasive discrimination, prejudice, harassment, or violence, this clearly represents a deeply problematic trend that is co-constituted with straight male sexuality (cf. Anderson 2009). Straight male transphobia, even limited to the common unwillingness in straight men to date or have sex with trans women (especially when still possessing a penis [Weinberg and Williams 2010]), is therefore a serious problem that results in exclusionary and potentially even violent behaviors against trans people (Lombardi et al. 2002).
Ultimately, there are few, if any, non-transphobic/hysteric reasons for straight men to exclude trans women from their dating and sexual interests. Still, the most common fixates upon the trans woman’s genitals, especially when she has a penis (cf. Weinberg and Williams 2010). Indeed, the trans woman’s penis, when she has one, is the specific site of much straight male transhysteric anxiety, as though hegemonic masculinity demands straight men form partnerships only with vaginas, not with women. Ultimately, this concern for the transhysteric male includes the performance of sex acts deemed demasculinizing only because they involve another penis, even when that penis is part of a woman’s body. Among these sexual acts, of unavoidable concern for the transhysteric straight male is the potential threat of receptive anal eroticism from his trans partner if she desires to penetrate him. In this way, transhysteria presents a significant difference from homohysteria: the latter is a paranoia about the self while the former is about the other. Thus, straight male concerns related to receptive penetrative anal eroticism may directly contribute to the problem of straight male transphobia. This suggests that reducing anxiety about receptive penetrative anal eroticism among straight males may ameliorate transphobia in the same.
Branfman et al. (2017) point out, however, that while these are suggestive signs, there also may be limitations to this avenue to transformative politics. They note, As Branfman and Ekberg-Stiritz (2012) have written, exploring anal pleasure does not automatically transform straight men’s politics or their treatment of sexual and gender minorities. In fact, if anal pleasure does indeed lose its cultural association with homosexuality and feminization, this decoupling may simply free straight men to explore it without critically questioning their own oppressive beliefs about gender and sexuality. (p. 15, italics added).
It is upon these grounds that they make the recommendations that inform this study:
[W]e suggest that the destigmatization of anal pleasure at least has the potential to open space for critical questions and dialogues about gender and sexual orientation that would previously have been silenced. From a social justice standpoint, we believe that even as decreased homophobia opens space for straight men to experiment with previously homosexualizing activities like anal eroticism, so in turn may increased understanding of anal pleasure help reduce stigma projected onto gay, bisexual, and other queer-identified men. In openly discussing all men’s capacity for anal pleasure, receptivity, and penetrability, young men might question accepted gender norms and the stigmas of emasculation, deviance, and dirtiness that are so often used to degrade it. (p. 15).
It is in light of these observations and aspirations, including challenging the orthodoxies of heterosexuality (see Beasley et al. 2015; Paasonen 2017) and investigating the limitations of and challenges to inclusive masculinity, that the present qualitative study was conducted.
Methodology
This study utilizes long-form, semi-structured, in-person casual interviews to gather qualitative data on thirteen men about their relationship with receptive anal eroticism and attitudes regarding the relevant co-constitutive variables discussed above. Interviews were conducted at locations of the subjects’ choosing in or around Portland, Oregon, between the dates of August 15 and November 6, 2017. Questions were designed to facilitate open-ended discussions about the variables under examination, while offering the researcher a subjective feel for the individual subjects and their views.
In conducting this study, I interviewed eight straight, two bisexual, and three gay men about their experience with receptive anal eroticism, sex toys, history of and openness to (anal) sex toy use, and topics relevant to other factors under consideration. Though the subject of the study is straight-male attitudes, gay and bisexual men were included to obtain diverse insights from the perspectives of those with other orientations. The sample is small and naturally limits the breadth of conclusions that can be drawn; however, it provides exploratory indications into the ways sexual orientation may interact with these factors.
Given the intimate nature of the interviews and the subject matter, subjects were recruited through word-of-mouth via a snowball sampling method extending from a social network in Portland, Oregon, in which I am situated. That is, I recruited acquaintances and partners of acquaintances and, consistent with snowball sampling, they recruited volunteers from within their social networks. To minimize bias, I recruited few of my own acquaintances and relied more heavily on snowball sampling while specifically seeking diversity of (political) views, as this is known to be associated with attitudes about (male) anality (cf. Pacilli et al. 2011; Terrizzi et al. 2010), and anal sexual experience.
Most interviews lasted ninety minutes to two hours and were based upon fifteen core questions, though subjects were encouraged to wander naturally through the processes of conversation. Subjects were given free range to talk about the core questions and any associations that consequently arose. This semi-structured conversational interview technique was chosen because it enabled applying a combined naturalist and constructivist grounded theory approach to the qualitative data that emerged (Rubin and Rubin 2012, 8–11, 17–26). As McKee (2014) points out, qualitative methodologies can provide more validity (at the cost of reliability) than quantitative methods allow by avoiding reducing subjects to a set of answers and numerical data, as sometimes limits quantitative and other qualitative methodologies. This conversational approach also added an ethnographic element, which benefits from closeness, though it can also be limited by subjective blurring. To correct for this, critical detachment was utilized during postinterview note compilation (per Matthews 2014, 105–106). Finally, the data were analyzed thematically for indications of trans/homohysteria/phobia (Anderson 2008), feminist attitudes, attitudes about rape and rape culture, partner sensitivity, and inclusive versus hegemonic masculinity (Anderson 2009; Connell 1995).
Interviews began by asking the age and relationship status of the participants. They continued by engaging the following fifteen core questions, which were presented as prompts for a semi-structured approach. The research informing these questions includes the immediately aforementioned together with considerations of their relevance to the topic (in some cases, additional relevant literature is cited at the end of the question):
- How would you describe your sexual orientation: straight, gay, bisexual, or something else?
- How would you describe your political orientation?
- How do you feel about sex toys as a part of masturbation, sex, or sexual play?
- Do you feel that certain sex toys are suited to particular genders, e.g., are dildos toys for women? Which are which, and how do you tell? (cf. Watson et al. 2015)
- Have you ever used a penetrative sex toy on yourself in masturbation or sexual play? Have you ever had one used on you by a partner during sexual play? Is this a regular part of your sexual play? Do you like it? (cf. Allan 2016b; Watson et al. 2015)
- Do you feel that if a man inserts a dildo into his own anus, or has one used on him as such by a partner (e.g., pegging), it robs him of or otherwise damages his masculinity? Does it enhance his masculinity? Or is it unrelated to masculinity? (cf. Allan 2016b; Anderson 2009; Branfman and Ekberg-Stiritz 2012; Connell 1995)
- Do you feel like there are gendered roles in sex, i.e., “male” and “female” roles? To the degree it is applicable, do you feel like you take on more “male” roles or “female” ones? (cf. Glickman and Emirzian 2013)
- Would you describe yourself as masculine? In what ways? (cf. Anderson 2009; Connell 1995)
- Do you think rape culture is a significant problem in society today? (cf. Baker 2015)
- Should we believe victims of sexual assault, especially rape? (cf. Baker 2015)
- Do you think your partner(s) would describe you as a “sensitive guy”? Would you describe yourself this way? (cf. Glickman and Emirzian 2013)
- Are you a feminist? How do you feel about feminism? (cf. Houvouras and Carter 2008)
- Would you describe yourself as homophobic? Would others describe you as homophobic? (Anderson 2009)
- Would you date and/or have sex with a trans woman? Would it matter to you and your sexual satisfaction if she has a penis? (If straight), would you say this is consistent with being straight and masculine? (cf. Weinberg and Williams 2010)
- Would your willingness to date and/or have sex with a trans woman, especially one with a penis, increase if you were more comfortable with receptive penetrative anal eroticism? (cf. Glickman and Emirzian 2013; Weinberg and Williams 2010)
Recording and transcribing such interviews is standard practice, though exceptions exist for when there are reasons to conclude that the act of recording may negatively influence the subjects or data validity (Nordstrom 2015; Rubin and Rubin 2012; Speer and Hutchby 2003; Suzuki et al. 2007). In this case, two subjects refused to participate if recorded because they found the subject matter sensitive and three others expressed discomfort. Thus, to encourage openness, and in the interest of fair and ethical treatment across all subjects, interviews were not recorded (Rubin and Rubin 2012, 35, cf. 178). Instead, extensive, detailed typed notes were kept during interviews and post-interview logging and analysis was immediate and engaged critical detachment (Matthews 2014, 105–106; Rubin and Rubin 2012, 64). All direct quotations were noted as they were spoken and confirmed by subjects for accuracy.
Responses
Interview subjects included eight straight men (Subjects 1–8), two bisexual men (Subjects 9–10), and three homosexual men (Subjects 11–13), all of whom are open about their sexualities. No subjects reported an orientation outside of these three categories. Subjects’ ages ranged from twenty-seven to fifty-four. Among the straight men, five (Subjects 1, 2, 3, 4, 7) described themselves as partners in stable long-term relationships, two (Subjects 5, 6) classified themselves as “dating around,” and one (Subject 8) was single. Both bisexual men were in a committed long-term “lightly poly[amorous]” relationship—with each other—and one maintained a “casual but ongoing” relationship with a woman outside the same-sex dyad. One gay man (Subject 11) was involved in a long-term relationship, while the other two were single. Henceforth, in referencing subjects, when clarity is needed the following format will be used: “Subject X[AO],” with X marking subject number, A representing his age, and O his sexual orientation (as S, B, or G, denoting straight, bisexual, or gay, respectively). For example, “Subject 1[31S]” indicates that Subject 1 is being referenced, and he is 31 years old and straight.
In order to minimize a potential bias in the study, I expressly aimed to recruit diversity in the political orientations of the interview subjects via the snowball sampling, as it is known to be associated with beliefs connected with male anality (Pacilli et al. 2011; Terrizzi et al. 2010). Despite screening some potential subjects for this reason, the political orientations of participants still skew generally toward what would be described as “progressive” on sociosexual issues. Only four of the thirteen subjects self-described by terms other than “liberal” or “progressive”; three used “libertarian” (Subjects 2[36S], 3[29S], 5[41S]) and one (Subject 1[31S]) “socially conservative.” This last subject gave the briefest interview (lasting barely thirty minutes) and the briefest answers of all thirteen subjects. Of note, I invited six socially conservative men to participate, but five declined, stating that they, in the words of one, “[didn’t] want to be a part of some stupid liberal study about putting stuff up [one’s] butt.” This necessarily raises questions about how information of this kind could be obtained from socially conservative straight men. It also forces us to ask two questions about Eric Anderson’s (2009) theory of inclusive masculinity: For whom is masculinity inclusive? and (How) is political orientation connected to an inclusive-versus-hegemonic-masculinity divide?
All subjects expressed generally positive attitudes toward sex toys as a part of masturbation, sex, and sexual play, and all felt that certain sex toys are better suited for particular genders. There was, however, more knowing laughter and a broader range of male-suitable toys amongst gay and bisexual subjects. While four straight subjects (Subjects 3[29S], 4[54S], 6[34S], 8[52S]) stated that the intended gender for a sex toy is ambiguous in the case of penetrative toys, indicating openness to receptive anal eroticism and penetration, straight subjects generally believed sex toys for men are meant to be penetrated rather than penetrating (these including masturbators, sleeves, and molded vaginas). The first association all subjects had with dildos, for instance, was that they are specifically a woman’s sex toy. All but two subjects (Subjects 1[31S], 5[41S]), however, immediately qualified that identification. In the words of Subject 2[36S], “anal play makes it less clear-cut on who can use a dildo.” One subject, the socially conservative Subject 1[31S], expressed a problem even with the notion that any men might enjoy anally penetrating themselves with sex toys.
Two gay subjects (Subjects 11[40G] and 13[33G]), both bisexual men, and two straight subjects (Subjects 4[54S] and 8[52S]) intentionally had used or regularly use penetrative sex toys for masturbatory anal eroticism. Two further straight subjects (Subjects 3[29S], 6[34S]) had “tried it once to see if there was anything they were missing out on.” Only three subjects (Subjects 8[52S], 10[34B], 11[40G]) regularly masturbated with dildos. Among the four straight subjects who had not used a dildo on themselves (Subjects 1[31S], 2[36S], 5[41S], 7[27S]), two mentioned being willing to try it under the right circumstances (Subjects 2, 5) while two adamantly refused (Subjects 1, 7). Among the latter two, Subject 1[31S] (the social conservative) immediately associated it with homosexuality while Subject 7[27S] called the use of dildos for anal eroticism “gross.” As he emphatically phrased it, “That hole is exit-only! There’s poop up there!” Subject 6[34S], who had tried anally penetrating himself but had done so only once, remarked, “I didn’t like it much, frankly. I had had high hopes, but it mostly just felt like taking a shit that never ended and wasn’t really pleasurable.” The general consensus of all subjects except three (Subjects 8[52S], 9[43B], 12[42G]) is that male masturbators/sleeves constitute a vastly superior male sex toy experience as compared to penetrative toys.
These attitudes frequently matched thematic assessment of subjects’ self-identification with occupying masculine gender roles (cf. Connell 1995; Connell and Messerschmidt 2005), including expressions of the opinion that using penetrative anal sex toys on oneself diminishes a man’s masculinity (cf. Allan 2016b; Branfman et al. 2017; Connell 1995). Most straight subjects (Subjects 1–6) and one gay subject (Subject 12[42G]) expressed this perspective in greater or lesser degree.
The interview data delineated less obvious relationships between anal dildo use and attitudes about rape culture and feminism. There was, however, some apparent connection between these features within the data: those more engaged in self-mediated penetrative anal play seemed generally more feminist and more concerned about rape culture than others. For example, Subject 8[52S], who consistently expressed staunchly feminist attitudes, was also the most open to and enthusiastic about receptive penetrative anal play. He highlighted this connection with his remark, “I play with anal toys a lot and experiment a lot. I’ve even hurt myself with toys that are too big or too dry.” When asked if this bears any associations with rape for him, he commented that he has thought about it frequently and said, “Yeah, it can make you realize how bad a rape has to be, especially anally.” Views about feminism were generally supportive but somewhat mixed, except in Subjects 1[31S] and 5[41S], who were also the most socially conservative. All subjects expressed strongly negative views on rape, but six (Subjects 1[31S], 3[29S], 5[41S], 6[34S], 9[43B], 10[34B]) doubted the pervasiveness of rape culture. Other than the social conservative (Subject 1[31S]), both bisexual subjects and those straight subjects who had tried dildos in the past but do not use them regularly held the most skeptical attitudes about rape culture. The socially conservative Subject 1[31S] unambiguously indicated the most overtly negative attitudes about receptive anal eroticism, and these co-presented with strong negative attitudes about feminism, which he called “man-hating,” and rape culture, which he labelled a “liberal myth.”
Regarding the perception of themselves as homophobic (Question 13), the gay and bisexual subjects merely laughed at being asked the question “because I’m not straight” (Subject 9[43B]), and all eight straight subjects denied it, including the social conservative—indicating significant cultural inroads of inclusive masculinities. Through careful probing with follow-up questions, it became clear that Subjects 1[31S], 2[36S], 3[29S] and 5[41S] appear, as revealed through thematic analysis of language about non-straight orientations (cf. Braun and Clarke 2012, 62–68), more homophobic than they are willing to admit. This (latent) homophobia presented alongside clear trends of having not anally penetrated themselves. Surprisingly, the one straight subject for whom this pattern did not fit (Subject 7[27S]) was also unwilling to try receptive dildo eroticism. He exhibited no signs of homohysteria unless one counts the unwillingness to be anally penetrated, for which he attributed his aversion entirely to “hygiene.”
The most interesting and varied responses came from discussing trans women. Only five subjects (Subjects 4[54S], 8[52S], 9[43B], 10[34B], 13[33G]) were clearly willing to date or have sex with a trans woman, demonstrating a limitation of the cultural penetration of inclusive masculinity. Upon specifying that the trans woman has a penis, Subject 13[33G] remarked, “Of course! That would be requirement!” Neither bisexual subject expressed any hesitance about dating a trans woman with a penis or one without, though Subject 10[34B] (the one not also dating a woman) mentioned he would slightly prefer if she has one. The socially conservative Subject 1[31S] not only refused to date a trans woman “under any circumstances” but closed this topic of conversation after responding transphobically that trans women “are not women” and “suffer mental illness.” When queried about trans women with penises, he immediately insisted that “so-called ‘ladyboys’ and ‘chicks with dicks’ are not women; they’re dudes with a mental problem.” He even refused to entertain the topic hypothetically, calling it “messed up.” Subject 4[54S], who would date a trans woman, said he would not do so if she had a penis, thereby indicating the genitals/penis to be central to his transhysteric concerns. Subject 8[52S], on the other hand, indicated no particular preference and described himself as “up for anything, so long as it’s fun.” In stark contrast, both gay subjects who were unwilling to date or have sex with a trans woman (Subjects 11[40G], 12[42G]) indicated their sexuality as the reason. In the words of Subject 11, “Why would I? She’s a woman, and I’m a gay man.” This response does not necessarily indicate transphobia/hysteria, as it recognizes a trans woman as being a woman. However, their preference may still be transphobic, trans-exclusionary, and genital/penis-centric, since while these two subjects expressed no qualitative difference regarding whether or not the trans woman has a penis, Subject 12[42G] would not date a trans man and Subject 11[40G] only would if he had a functional post-operative penis.
Insightfully, regarding the roots of transhysteria/phobia, Subjects 2[36S], 3[29S], and 6[34S] indicated negative attitudes toward dating a trans woman generally, yet willingness “to consider it,” in the words of Subject 3[29S], “depending upon the woman.” For these subjects, however, the presence of a penis was unacceptable, reinforcing the notion that transhysteria/phobia may largely be other-centered and penis/genital-centric. As Subject 2[36S] summarized, “A penis is non-negotiable for me; it’s an absolute deal-breaker, no matter how nice, no matter how attractive, no matter what. Just no.” Subjects 3[39S], 6[34S], 7[27S], and 8[52S], however, were more open to dating or having sex with a trans woman, including one with a penis. This, still, seemed penis/genital-centric and may have to do with their willingness to engage in (and enjoy) regular receptive penetrative anal eroticism, at least in the hypothetical. As Subject 3[29S] put it, “I guess if I was used to it and liked getting it up the butt from someone, I could probably enjoy it from a trans woman if I found her really hot. But it would still be weird.”
Discussing trans women’s penises consistently created the most apparent discomfort for (most of) the (trans/homohysteric) straight subjects. Subject 6[34S], who had tried receptive dildo eroticism but did not enjoy it, remained unambiguously opposed to dating or having sex with a trans woman, especially if she had a penis. Subject 8[52S], who was “up for anything” and who regularly engages receptive dildo eroticism by himself and with (female) partners, was enthusiastically open to the possibility of sex with a trans woman with or without a penis, provided only that he was “into the woman” and that she was “good at what she does.” He remarked that trans women with penises are “very likely to qualify” and that he would seek out such potential sex partners if possible.
Discussion
These qualitative data offer suggestive tentative conclusions about the hypotheses posed here. For example, homo/transhysteria seems to explain some of the observed resistance to receptive penetrative anal eroticism (cf. Branfman et al. 2017), which may be constituted with conforming to hegemonic masculinity and a limitation to inclusive masculinity in anally non-receptive men (cf. Connell 1995). Emergent themes in the data indicate that self-perceived importance of adhering to hegemonic masculine gender roles over inclusive ones was strongly aligned with this attitude. It also appears connected with self-perceptions of not being sensitive in a partnered setting. Of course, although such qualitative data can provide high validity, it places limits on reliability (McKee 2014), and the small sample size limits the strength of these suggestive observations.
This qualitative assessment also suggests the conclusion that there may be significant connections between the masculine trope of “doing sex,” feeling oneself masculine, and resistance to anal receptivity, as Glickman and Emirzian (2013) note and seek to challenge (cf. Allan 2016b; Connell 1995). Specifically, men who reported penetrating themselves anally most frequently and who derive the most pleasure from it seemed to understand receptive sexual roles better than their anal non-receptive peers and were more likely to self-describe as feminists. Similarly, consistent with Glickman and Emirzian (2013), receptive penetrative anal eroticism may improve partner sensitivity in men, especially straight men. With only one exception (Subject 3[29S]), subjects who self-assessed and thematically exhibited the most partner sensitivity were also those most aware of (and able to enjoy) what it feels like to receive sexual penetration. Future research taking partners’ evaluations of sensitivity into account would be valuable.
Where it comes to sensitivity about rape culture, the data are barely suggestive, and this ambiguity between theory and thematically coded results provides an avenue for future research. In that the sole social conservative in the study was also the most hostile to feminism and the problem of rape culture, the question of the remedial potential for receptive anal eroticism in straight men to improve views about feminism, rape, and rape culture remains hopeful but mostly open, despite Allan’s (2016b, 177, 186) cautious hesitance. Particularly, exploring socially conservative perspectives concerning penetrative anal eroticism and issues surrounding rape would benefit from further detailed consideration.
On the co-constituted topics of transphobia/hysteria, however, the data were the most clearly suggestive. The apparent relationship between sexual orientation and transphobia/hysteria seems clear, noting limitations from the small sample size. Straight subjects exhibited far more transphobic attitudes and transhysteria (especially in being unwilling to date trans women with penises) than the gay or bisexual subjects. Furthermore, receptivity to penetrative anal eroticism consistently arose in the least transphobic/hysterical subjects. Bolstering this conclusion, subjects who presented transphobic/hysterical themes expressed that receptive penetrative anal eroticism is likely to evoke less-transphobic attitudes for them, at least in the hypothetical scenario of receptivity to sex with a trans woman with a penis. Overall, the clear thematic trend across most straight subjects is that potentially transhysteric/phobic attitudes in straight men seem penis/genitalcentric, suggesting that receiving another’s penis still represents a (hegemonic?) barrier to inclusive masculinities. These data are therefore suggestive that education, destigmatization, and encouragement of receptive penetrative anal eroticism in straight men could partially remediate some significant problems under the transphobic umbrella and may help counter penis/genital-centric transhysteria in straight men.
In conclusion, this qualitative study suggests that men who report greater comfort with receptive penetrative anal eroticism also report less transphobia, less obedience to masculine gender norms, greater partner sensitivity, and potentially greater awareness about rape. Particularly, unwillingness to engage in receptive penetrative anal eroticism seems to arise most commonly (especially in straight men) alongside masculinity features such as heteronormativity, hegemonic masculinity, and trans/ homohysteria. This seems to mark a penis-centered limitation to inclusive masculinity and may represent an inherent tension between hegemonic and inclusive masculinity that may relate to political orientation. Particularly from this data, transphobia and transhysteria in straight men exhibit such a close thematic relationship to receptivity to penetrative anal eroticism that intentional anal penetration in “safe” environments may remediate them.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Conflict of interest: The author declares that he/she has no conflict of interest.
Ethical Approval: All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institution and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments, including standard IRB approval which was obtained for this study.
Human and Animal Rights: This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by the author
Informed Consent: Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
References
Allan, J. A. (2016a). Phallic affect, or why men’s rights activists have feelings. Men and Masculinities, 19(1), 22–41.
Allan, J. A. (2016b). Reading from behind: A cultural analysis of the Anus. Regina, SK: University of Regina Press.
Anderson, E. (2008). “Being masculine is not about who you sleep with…”: Heterosexual athletes contesting masculinity and the one-time rule of homosexuality. Sex Roles, 58(1–2), 104–115.
Anderson, E. (2009). Inclusive masculinity: The changing nature of masculinities. London: Routledge.
Attwood, F. (2005). Fashion and passion: Marketing sex to women. Sexualities, 8(4), 392–406.
Baker, M. L. (2015). Sexism, masculinity, and entitlement as predictors of rape culture support, and the role of empathy as a possible explanation. Senior Independent Study Thesis, Paper 6651, The College of Wooster, Wooster, OH.
Beasley, C., Holmes, M., & Brook, H. (2015). Heterodoxy: Challenging orthodoxies about heterosexuality. Sexualities, 18(5–6), 681–697.
Blozendahl, C. I., & Myers, D. J. (2004). Feminist attitudes and support for gender equality: Opinion change in women and men, 1974–1998. Social Forces, 83, 759–789.
Blue, V. (2007). The adventurous couples’ guide to strap-on sex. San Francisco, CA: Cleis Press.
Branfman, J., & Ekberg-Stiritz, S. (2012). Teaching men’s anal pleasure: Challenging gender norms with “prostage” education”. American Journal of Sexuality Education, 7(4), 404–428.
Branfman, J., Stiritz, S., & Anderson, E. (2017). Relaxing the straight male anus: Decreasing homohysteria around anal eroticism. Sexualities. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1363460716678560.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2012). Thematic analysis. In H. M. Cooper (Ed.), APA handbook of research methods in psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 57–71). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Burke, K. (2014). What makes a man: Gender and sexual boundaries on evangelical Christian sexuality websites. Sexualities, 17(1–2), 3–22.
Canan, S. N., Jozkowski, K. N., & Crawford, B. L. (2016). Sexual assault supportive attitudes: Rape myth acceptance and token resistance in Greek and non-Greek college students from two university samples in the United States. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0886260516636064.
Carrera, M. V., DePalma, R., & Lameiras, M. (2012). Sex/gender identity: Moving beyond fixed and “natural” categories. Sexualities, 15(8), 995–1016.
Carrillo, H., & Hoffman, A. (2017). “Straight with a pinch of bi”: The construction of heterosexuality as an elastic category among adult US men. Sexualities, 21(1–2), 90–108.
Connell, R. W. (1995). Masculinities. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Connell, R. W., & Messerschmidt, J. W. (2005). Hegemonic masculinity: Rethinking the concept. Gender & Society, 19(6), 829–859.
Foa, E. B., Dancu, C. V., Hembree, E. A., Jaycox, L. H., Meadows, E. A., & Street, G. P. (1999). A comparison of exposure therapy, stress inoculation training, and their combination for reducing posttraumatic stress disorder in female assault victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67(2), 194–200.
Frank, K. (2008). Not gay, but not homophobic”: Male sexuality and homophobia in the “lifestyle. Sexualities, 11(4), 435–454.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56, 109–118.
Glickman, C., & Emirzian, A. (2013). The ultimate guide to prostate pleasure: Erotic exploration for men and their partners. New York: Cleis Press.
Hayes, R. M., Abbott, R. L., & Cook, S. (2016). It’s her fault: Student acceptance of rape myths on two college campuses. Violence Against Women, 22(13), 1540–1555.
Heywood, W., & Smith, A. M. A. (2014). Anal sex practices in heterosexual and male homosexual populations: A review of population-based data. Sexual Health, 9(6), 517–526.
Houvouras, S., & Carter, J. S. (2008). The F word: College students’ definitions of a feminist. Sociological Forum, 23(2), 234–256.
Kimmel, M. (2001). Masculinity as homophobia: Fear, shame, and silence in the construction of gender identity. In S. Whitehead & F. Barrett (Eds.), The masculinities reader (pp. 266–287). Cambridge, MA: Polity Press.
Lombardi, E. L., Wilchins, R. A., Priesing, D., & Malouf, D. (2002). Gender violence: Transgender experience with violence and discrimination. Journal of Homosexuality, 42(1), 89–101.
Matthews, C. R. (2014). Biology ideology and pastiche hegemony. Men and Masculinities, 17(2), 99–119.
McBride, K. R. (2017). Heterosexual women’s anal sex attitudes and motivations: A focus group study. The Journal of Sex Research. https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00224499.2017.1355437?journalCode=hjsr20.
McKee, A. (2014). Humanities and social scientific research methods in porn studies. Porn Studies, 1(1–2), 53–63.
McNeil, T. (2013). Sex education and the promotion of heteronormativity. Sexualities, 16(7), 826–846.
Nagoshi, J. L., Adams, K. A., Terrell, H. K., Hill, E. D., Brzuzy, S., & Nagoshi, C. T. (2008). Gender differences in correlates of homophobia and transphobia. Sex Roles, 59, 521–531.
Nordstrom, S. N. (2015). Not so innocent anymore: Making recording devices matter in qualitative interviews. Qualitative Inquiry, 21(4), 388–401.
Osman, S. L. (2016). Predicting rape victim empathy based on rape victimization and acknowledgement labeling. Violence Against Women, 22(7), 767–779.
Paasonen, S. (2017). Many splendored things: Sexuality, playfulness and play. Sexualities. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1363460717731928.
Pacilli, M. G., Taurino, A., Jost, J. T., & van der Toorn, J. (2011). System justification, right-wing conservatism, and internalized homophobia: Gay and lesbian attitudes toward same-sex parenting in Italy. Sex Roles, 65(7–8), 580–595.
Pascoe, C. J. (2007). Dude, you’re a fag: Adolescent masculinity and the fag discourse. Sexualities, 8(3), 329–346.
Phillips, T. (2017). Campus rape culture: Effects on individual, social, and administrative levels. Honor’s thesis, Baylor University, Waco, TX. Retrieved from https://baylor-ir.tdl.org/handle/2104/9973. Accessed 1 June 2018.
Pronger, B. (1998). On your knees: Carnal knowledge, masculine dissolution, doing feminism. In T. Digby (Ed.), Men Doing Feminism (pp. 69–79). New York: Routledge.
Pronger, B. (1999). “Outta my endzone”: Sport and the territorial anus. Journal of Sport & Social Issues, 23(4), 373–389.
Reling, T. T., Barton, M. S., Becker, S., & Valasik, M. A. (2017). Rape myths and hookup culture: An exploratory study of U.S. college students’ perceptions. Sex Roles. https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11199-017-0813-4.
Rentschler, C. A. (2014). Rape culture and the feminist politics of social media. Girlhood Studies, 7(1), 65–82.
Rubin, H. J., & Rubin, I. S. (2012). Qualitative Interviewing: The Art of Hearing Data (3rd ed.). Los Angeles: SAGE.
Savin-Williams, R. C., & Vrangalova, Z. (2013). Mostly heterosexual as a distinct sexual orientation group: A systematic review of the empirical evidence. Developmental Review, 33(1), 58–88.
Schrock, D. P., & Padavic, I. (2007). Negotiating hegemonic masculinity in a batterer intervention program. Gender & Society, 21(5), 625–649.
Silva, T. (2017). Bud-sex: Constructing normative masculinity among rural straight men that have sex with men. Gender & Society, 31(1), 51–73.
Speer, S. A., & Hutchby, I. (2003). From ethics to analytics: Aspects of participants’ orientations to the presence and relevance of recording devices. Sociology, 37(2), 315–337.
Suzuki, L. A., Ahluwalia, M. K., Arora, A. K., & Mattis, J. S. (2007). The pond you fish in determines the fish you catch: Exploring strategies for qualitative data collection. The Counseling Psychologist, 35(2), 295–327.
Terrizzi, J. A., Jr., Shook, N. J., & Ventis, W. L. (2010). Disgust: A predictor of social conservatism and prejudicial attitudes toward homosexuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(6), 587–592.
Ward, J. (2015). Not gay: Sex between straight white men. New York: NYU Press.
Watson, E. D., Séguin, L. J., Milhausen, R. R., & Murray, S. H. (2015). The impact of a couple’s vibrator on men’s perceptions of their own and their partner’s sexual pleasure and satisfaction. Men and Masculinities, 19(4), 370–383.
Weinberg, M. S., & Williams, C. J. (2010). Men sexually interested in transwomen (MSTW): Gendered embodiment and the construction of sexual desire. Journal of Sex Research, 47(4), 374–383.
Reviewer Comments
A history of relevant communications about the “Dildos” paper
First decision: May 7, 2018
Dear Ms. Smith,
Thank you again for submitting your manuscript, “Going in Through the Back Door: Challenging Straight Male Homohysteria and Transphobia through Receptive Penetrative Sex Toy Use”, to Sexuality & Culture. The manuscript has been peer reviewed.
Based on the advice received, your manuscript can be given further consideration for publication should you be prepared to incorporate major revisions. When preparing your revised manuscript, you are asked to carefully consider the reviewer comments attached below and also to submit a list of responses to the comments. Your list of responses should be uploaded as a file separate from the revised manuscript.
In order to submit your revised manuscript electronically, please access the following web site:
[REDACTED]Your username is: [REDACTED]If you forgot your password, you can click the ‘Send Login Details’ link on the login page.
Please click “Author Login” to submit your revision.
I look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.
Best regards,
[REDACTED], PhDEditor-in-Chief
Sexuality & Culture
Comments to the Author
Reviewer #1:
This article is an incredibly rich and exciting contribution to the study of sexuality and culture, and particularly the intersection between masculinity and anality. In what follows, I provide a few suggestions, some questions, and general (and curious) inquiries. The goal of these comments is to help the author develop their argument, rather than to be used to discount the contribution. This contribution, to be certain, is important, timely, and worthy of publication.
In the opening sections, the author notes that “though Allan lays out psychoanalytic theoretical considerations that are strongly suggestive of the co-constitutive relationship between masculinity, the variables listed above, and anality, currently there is no scholarly literature that engages the topic of straight male penetrative sex toy directly and substantively” (3). The author here is referring to Allan’s article, “Phallic Affect,” however, Allan’s book, Reading from Behind: A Cultural Analysis of the Anus (2016) might prove to be more useful in the context of this study.
The author writes that “there exists a far more extensive and applicable treatment in the book, The Ultimate Guide to Prostate Pleasure, but unfortunately this insightful volumes falls considerably outside of the scholarly academic canon” (3). I’m not certain that this is a problem, perhaps this is a difference of approach, but it seems to me that sex manuals are highly valuable resources in scholarly work and if there is a problem that the problem rests not with The Ultimate Guide but the Academy’s inability to imagine value outside of itself. Indeed, the author might consider expanding this to include books like, The Adventurous Couple’s Guide to Strap-On Sex by Violet Blue.
The use of Anderson’s notions of “homosexualization” and “homohysteria” are interesting and important. However, the author might want to consider reading further into Anderson’s work, for instance, he has revised his argument about the “one-time rule” (6), particularly in later work. For Anderson, it seems that there is no longer a “one-time rule,” but an increasing acceptance of what Savin-Williams calls “mostly straight,” as does Mark McCormack in his recent work.
As I read this paper, something that occurs to me is that there is a difference between rimming and penetration. How do these men imagine the tickling of the anus, pleasuring the perineum, etc.? Is it that penetration alone is the problem? Or is everything anal problematic? Obviously, this is going to depend on the men, but what of levels of anality? These questions become particularly important when the
author writes, “that is, for many straight men, anal penetration is something that they do (to a woman) but not something that can be done to them (by anyone, man, woman, or self)” (7). I am particularly interested here in “self.” If not a sex toy, then a finger? I know the study is focused in particular on sex toys, but what about just simple anal exploration.
The inclusion of the “mostly straight” on page 8 is a welcome contribution. Perhaps also consider including Jane Ward’s work here, notably, Not Gay: Sex Between Straight White Men (2015).
The author writes, “the research literature on sex and sexuality has been surprisingly inattentive to sex toy use vis-à-vis straight men, as most are situated toward women’s issues” (9). Is it just straight men or is it men more generally? There is a lot at stake in not thinking about men and sex toys, what does this say about scholars working at the intersections of masculinity and sexuality?
I’m not sure that I am convinced by the claim that “it is therefore arguable that encouraging receptive penetrative anal eroticism in straight men can increase sensitivity to rape victimization and rape culture
by overcoming masculinity-based resistance in bystanders” (11). It is certainly “arguable,” but what would “proof” look like in this scenario? I think it is true that there is “virtually no rigorous scholarly work [that] investigates the topic of improving straight male partner sensitivity by means of receptive anal eroticism,” but I do wonder what such a study would look like. Right now, it seems, that much of this is speculative, but nonetheless interesting and provocative.
One aspect of this paper that I am very curious about and where I see a great deal of potential is the braiding together of Anderson’s “homohysteria” with “transphobia.” I’m thinking here about the “unwillingness for straight men to date or have sex with transwomen,” (13) is this a kind of transhysteria, not about the self, but about the other? I’m not certain if Anderson has discussed any of this in his work. Attached to this, then, is why would men engage in sex with one another (as Ward, Silva, Savin-Williams and others might have it), but not with trans women? Perhaps beyond the scope of this paper, is this about patriarchy and dominance bonding (as Messner might have it)? Sorry for so many questions, but this paper is so rich and exciting, I’m just overwhelmed by so many new questions—which is a sign of a marvelous paper!
In thinking about the methodology, I’d like to see a bit more discussion around the socially conservative men invited to participate. Why would they decline to participate? This question is important because it begs the next question: how would one get information from these men? Attached to this, how does this affect Anderson’s theories about “inclusive masculinity”? For whom is masculinity inclusive? Does the kind of sex toy used make a difference? For instance, anal beads versus a dildo versus a buttplug.
Why did the gay and bisexual men “merely laugh at the being asked” (20) about homophobia? In thinking about the future of this paper, it might be worthwhile to speak with partners to assess “partner sensitivity in men” (23).
Overall, this paper is a very interesting contribution to knowledge. I think it might be beneficial to slow down and explore the transphobic elements of the study. New questions certainly arise about the nature of sexuality, for instance, is sexuality genitally-based rather than based on the person? These men seem discomforted by the trans woman with a penis, and it is this latter qualifier that shifts the nature of the discussion towards the genitalia rather than the person. The discussion section should include a larger consideration of future directions for research.
Reviewer #2:
Thank you for this exciting research. I enjoyed reading your paper, and I recommend publishing it after significant revisions. These revisions fall into three categories: 1) Clarity of your argument, 2) Theoretical explanations, and 3) Clarity of your phrasing.
Clarity of your argument:
In your abstract and throughout the paper, you say that the study explores how anal eroticism relates to many other issues (like transphobia or feminist consciousness). Until I read your discussion section, though, I did not understand exactly what you meant. Once I read that section, it seems like you’re asking, “Do men who report greater comfort with receptive anal pleasure also report less transphobia, less obedience to masculine gender norms, greater partner sensitivity, and greater awareness about rape?” If this is your question, I’d directly state it in the abstract and conclusion. This is clearer than saying that you explore how these themes are “associated.” Also related to clarity of your argument, it seems awfully hard to investigate so many topics from a single interview with such a small sample. I understand that qualitative research often uses small samples, but I think you should emphasize from the start that this sample does not enable you to find concrete correlations between these different issues. A more realistic claim for your study is that it suggests interesting correlations among these topics, and that other researchers should pursue additional studies to see how these correlations bear out. You address this somewhat toward the end, but it’s important to be upfront about the study limitations from the start–it makes the whole study more credible.
Theoretical explanations:
Your explanation of Anderson’s concepts (like homohysteria and homosexualization) on p.4 and p.6 isn’t quite right. On p.4, the definition you give for “homosexualization” is actually the definition that Anderson gives for “homohysteria.” Another important theoretical issue is hegemonic masculinity: You mention it many times, but I could not find a place where you explain the concept. Although many readers likely know about hegemonic masculinity, it’s important to at least briefly explain this concept and how it relates to your work. Connell’s work on hegemonic masculinity also includes some specific references to anal eroticism, and the way anal eroticism challenges hegemonic masculinity. Finally, in a few cases you mention that interviewees did or didn’t conform to a scale of masculine norms, but I could not find a place where you explain how you measured this. Did you use a pre-existing measure? If so, which one and how did you pick it?
Clarity of Phrasing:
This paper contains many run-on sentences. These run-ons appear in the abstract and throughout the whole article. Such long sentences are confusing to read, especially since they address many different topics within a single sentence. Likewise, many sentences in the article contain unnecessary extra phrases and/or grammatically awkward phrasing. I’m not saying this to upset you–very few articles have flawless phrasing on the first draft. I strongly advise you to work with a proofreader (like an academic colleague who isn’t an expert on your topic) to make sure that every sentence of your article is truly polished and clear, even to non-experts. This revision will help your argument to come across much more convincingly and accessibly.
Reviewer #3:
I appreciated having the opportunity to review a manuscript that focuses on an understudied aspect of male sexuality. While the topic is important, I have significant concerns about the research methodology, data analyses, results, and interpretations. In general, the author should address mechanisms for minimizing bias as the validity of the research is in question without that information. I am very concerned about the author’s tendency to make interpretative statements that could be perceived as judgmental. The repeated use of statistical terms (i.e. correlation, predictor) is inappropriate for a qualitative manuscript since the data cannot be used in this way. Below, I have provided several comments for consideration.
1. Page 15, line 44 addresses participants’ orientation. I am wondering if the author can further explain the rationale for including men that identified as bisexual and gay? Was this done in order to compare perspectives and experiences by orientation? If so, the sample size is quite small. A further discussion of this methodological choice would be valuable.
2. Page 15, line 54 states that participants were recruited through the author’s social network. What exactly is the nature of the social network? Did the investigator already know the participants? What steps were taken to minimize bias? A further discussion of these decisions is needed since the validity of the study rests, in part, on measures that were taken to control for bias.
3. Page 15, line 62 states that a conversational interview style was selected because it is a better representation of the subjects. Can the author provide a citation to support this assertion?
4. Page 16, line 20 describes the development of the interview guide. Was any theoretical framework used to inform the questions? Since there are several theories of masculinity, I am curious if any were applied. If not, can the author provide a rationale?
5. Page 16, line 27 presents interview questions. Some of the questions seem very leading and I am wondering why the author did not select a semi-structured or unstructured approach that would have allowed for ideas to emerge naturally?
6. Page 17, line 35 states that interviews were not recorded in order to encourage openness among participants. I am quite concerned about this aspect of the procedures and wonder if the author can provide citations to support this choice. Recording is standard and has been implemented across studies that deal with sensitive aspects of sexuality and behavior successfully.
7. Was the study approved by an IRB? I don’t see that information included. I would also like to know when data were collected and where.
8. Were subjects screened for anal sexual experience as either/both the receptive or insertive partner? There is evidence to suggest that behavioral experience can influence attitudes and perceptions. I am wondering if the author can address behavioral experience as a recruitment criteria.
9. Page 18, line 33 states that the politically conservative subject was the “most dismissive.” This seems to be a personal judgment by the researcher and not an objective measure. Can the author describe what about the answers were dismissive? I recommend caution in the interpretation of data.
10. Can the author provide a further discussion of why political orientation was assessed? There are data that address relationships been political beliefs and attitudes towards sexuality and behavior. Citing the literature would strengthen the rationale for assessing this variable.
11. The quotes reported in the results section would be strengthened by adding participants’ ages and orientations. It would ease interpretability and help contextualize the findings for the reader.
12. Page 19, line 30 states that the participant “scoffed” at the notion. I am concerned about the use of language that could be interpreted as judgmental.
13. Page 20, line 8 states that there was a slight correlation. Since no statistical analyses were conducted, the author cannot make this claim. In fact, qualitative methods aren’t intended to examine data using statistical concepts and making the analogy is a cause for concern.
14. Page 20, line 13 discusses rape culture and feminism but the illustrative quote that follows does not seem to have anything to do with those concepts. I am not sure what point the author was trying to make. Please clarify.
15. Page 20, line 56 references a “perfect correlation.” Again, correlation is a statistical analysis and cannot be applied to qualitative data. Please be cautious in the presentation of findings.
16. Page 20, line 54 states that subjects were more homophobic that they wanted to admit. Was this the author’s interpretation or was homophobia objectively measured? Many of the findings seemed to be
subjectively analyzed through the author’s lens. What was the analytic framework that was used? Can the author discuss that point in more detail and provide citations?
17. The discussion section is framed as if the study were quantitative and uses terms such a predictor, correlation, and mediator. These are not appropriate in the context of qualitative research and suggest a fundamental misunderstanding of qualitative approaches.
Acceptance by Sexuality & Culture: June 9, 2018
Dear Ms. Smith,
I am pleased to inform you that your manuscript “Going in Through the Back Door: Challenging Straight Male Homohysteria, Transhysteria, and Transphobia through Receptive Penetrative Sex Toy Use” has been accepted for publication in Sexuality & Culture. You will receive proofs prior to publication.
Please remember to include your manuscript number, # SECU-D-18-00036R1, whenever you inquire about your manuscript. Thank you.
Congratulations and best regards,
[REDACTED], PhDEditor-in-Chief
Sexuality & Culture